I developed the following piece at Carl Zimmer's Science Writing Workshop at Yale in February 2012.
It only takes a few minutes on Facebook and a few mindless clicks to get lost in a digital morass of vacation photos, party invitations, and wall posts. No longer is Facebook simply a tool for managing real-life social groups. It has become a platform for a new kind of socializing, where coworkers, acquaintances, and childhood playmates are lumped together in one group of ‘friends.’ These online social networks differ from real-life social networks in more ways than just the definition of friendship. They may actually utilize different social skills. In fact, a team of scientists led by Dr. Ryota Kanai of University College London discovered that this new brand of online socializing taps into different areas of the brain.
The human brain is specially adapted to navigate life in social groups. Scientists even know which brain regions are important for face recognition, empathy, and other social skills because of a recurring pattern between brain size and use. The brain controls the body with cells called neurons, which send rapid chemical signals to other neurons through a web of fibrous connections. Like wires on a switchboard, more connections arise among neurons that signal more often. A brain region that manages a particular skill will actually get larger the more that skill is practiced, because those neurons must form more connections to handle the increased signaling traffic. For instance, as real-life social networks get larger, so does an almond-shaped structure buried in the front of the brain. This structure, the amygdala, allows us to experience and perceive emotions, serving a critical role in managing complex social groups.
While scientists understand the relationship between the brain and real-life social networks, the biological basis behind online social networks remains unknown. Dr. Kanai and his team set out to solve this mystery, asking if differences in online network size can be explained by the size of known social brain regions, such as the amygdala. They predicted that if online and real-life socializing use the same skills, they would see similar trends in brain structure.
More than one hundred UCL students volunteered for the study. Each volunteer provided an MRI brain scan and reported his or her number of Facebook friends, which the scientists call the Facebook number. About half of the volunteers also answered a questionnaire about the size of their real-life social networks.
Using high-tech imaging tools, the scientists manipulated the brain scans, isolating the outer layer of the brain that is used in cognition and information processing – the grey matter. Just like any body part, individual brains are different sizes. The scientists accounted for these differences by adjusting the scale of the images, making every brain the same size. Then they spotted differences in the amount of grey matter found in each of the social brain regions. When they compared these amounts to the volunteers’ Facebook numbers, the results were eye-opening.
The scientists found that social brain regions do indeed get larger as online social networks get larger, but surprisingly, these are not the same regions that help us manage real-life networks. Three regions of the brain, each linked to socialization or memory, were larger in volunteers with larger Facebook numbers. However, the size of other social brain regions, including the amygdala, did not have a strong tie to the Facebook number. As expected, the amygdala was larger in volunteers with larger real-life networks, but the three regions that corresponded to Facebook number were not.
The team’s results suggest that the brain functions differently for online socializing than for real-life socializing. “There is a certain skill for online socializing that these areas subserve,” says Dr. Robert Ross, head of the Laboratory of Neurobiology at Fordham University. But without a strong link to the amygdala, online socializing could be missing the emotional dimension that we experience in real-life. “This may mean that you have developed the facility to distinguish truth from fiction, nuanced expression, all of that. But it doesn’t require any feeling,” says Ross. Dr. Kanai’s research demonstrates for the first time that online socializing is unique on a neurological level, and while it may complement real-life socializing, it’s no substitute for the real thing.
Kanai, R., B. Bahrami, R. Roylance, and G. Rees. 2011. Online social network size is reflected in human brain structure Proc. R. Soc. B April 7, 2012 279 (1732) 1327-1334
Image: detraveler.blogspot.com